Which of the following is the source of low self-control in the general theory of crime?

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC8095718

Annu Rev Criminol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2021 May 4.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC8095718

NIHMSID: NIHMS1594700

Abstract

Over the past several decades, Gottfredson & Hirschi’s (1990) SCT has dominated research on self-control and crime. In this review, I assess the current state of self-control knowledge and encourage the field to move beyond SCT, as its peculiar conceptualization of self-control and causal model presents challenges for integrative scholarship. Drawing heavily on scholarship outside criminology, I clarify the definition of self-control; describe the malleable nature of trait self-control; highlight its situational variability as state self-control; and consider the multiplicity of contextual, situational, and individual factors that affect its operation in relation to crime. This specification of contingencies and the interplay between impulse strength and control efforts in the process of self-control is intended as a springboard for research moving beyond SCT and its key premise that self-control ability is sufficient for explanation. Finally, I address what I see as important areas for further study in light of current knowledge.

Keywords: self-control, impulsivity, dual-influence model, control motivation

INTRODUCTION

Nearly 30 years ago, Gottfredson and Hirschi (G&H) published their self-control theory (SCT) in A General Theory of Crime (1990). Although not the first to focus on the role of internal controls in crime causation (e.g., Nye 1958; Reiss 1951) or to highlight the role of self-control on behavior (e.g., Anslie 1975; Logue 1988; Mischel et al. 1989; Rachlin 1974), G&H elevated the concept of self-control to the forefront of criminological thought and stimulated an extraordinary and sustained amount of research on self-control and crime. For illustration, A General Theory of Crime was the most widely cited work in the top 20 criminology journals in the first five years following its publication (Cohn and Farrington 1999); by the end of 2011, the book had been cited roughly 4,500 times (Google Scholar 12/30/2011); and this count more than doubled to 12 thousand cites by the end of 2018 (Google Scholar 12/30/2018). Hundreds of studies have explicitly tested elements of SCT, reporting widespread support for its central claim - that low self-control is a major cause of crime. This work has been covered in several authoritative reviews (Burt 2015; Goode 2008; Hay & Meldrum 2016); quantitative effect sizes have been estimated in meta-analyses (Pratt & Cullen 2000; Vazsonyi et al. 2017); and nary a month goes by without another published test of the theory. Self-control is widely, and largely uncontroversially, considered to be one of the strongest known correlates of crime1, a claim invariably referencing G&H’s text, now a criminological classic.

Indisputably, G&H’s theory has significantly contributed to the advancement of criminological knowledge. Yet, the success, even dominance, of SCT in criminology has tethered the field to a unique conception of self-control and causal model of its operation. Within criminology, SCT has gained a near-monopoly on self-control such that almost everything associated with self-control and crime is seen as falling under the purview of SCT. However, the past 30 years has also seen an explosion of research on self-control in other disciplines. This work has illuminated the multifaceted and dynamic nature of self-control processes, its developmental precursors and neurobiological underpinnings (e.g., Baumeister & Vohs 2004; Casey 2015; Hofmann et al. 2012; Steinberg et al. 2008; Vohs & Baumeister 2016). Unfortunately, the integration of these findings into criminology has been hindered by the field’s allegiance to SCT and its peculiar conception of self-control, theoretical assumptions and explanatory propositions, some of which have been clearly contradicted by empirical evidence. With the aim of overcoming impediments to scientific integration and advancing theorizing on self-control in criminology, this paper assesses the current state of self-control knowledge and encourages the field to move beyond SCT. Moving beyond SCT will enhance our ability to explain for whom, when, and how self-control is related to crime.

With this aim, this paper proceeds as follows. First, I briefly describe SCT and discuss (primarily criminological) research assessing the theory. Recognizing the existence of thorough reviews, I cover this voluminous scholarship relatively concisely, identifying key themes and referencing emblematic pieces. My focus is explicitly critical rather than laudatory, evaluating the empirical validity of SCT and detailing its explanatory limitations. Next, I turn to conceptual issues that present challenges to integration by confronting the “conceptual clutter” (Morrison & Grammer 2016) or so-called “jingle - jangle problem” (Kelley 1927; Block 1995) around self-control concepts. I propose relabeling G&H’s key construct and the adoption of a widely shared definition of self-control as the effortful inhibition of an impulse for immediate gratification in the service of long-term, higher-order goals (e.g., Duckworth & Steinberg 2015). In this view, self-control is a specific type of self-regulation involving a complex interplay of situational stimuli and individual dispositions. This reconceptualization requires uprooting empirically invalidated assumptions underlying SCT and replacing these untenable assumptions with insights from elaborated dual-influence models (e.g., Duckworth & Steinberg 2015; Metcalfe & Mischel 1999). Drawing primarily on scholarship outside criminology, I describe the relatively stable, yet malleable, nature of trait self-control; highlight its situational variability as state self-control; and consider the multiplicity of contextual, situational, and individual factors that affect the operation of self-control as it relates to crime. This specification of contingencies and motivational factors in the operation of self-control is intended as a jumping-off point for research moving beyond SCT and its key premise that self-control ability is sufficient for explanation. Finally, I address what I see as important areas for further study in light of current knowledge. Given space constraints, I cover a delimited amount of territory focusing on issues that I view as most pressing in this domain. Much more can and should be said, but I offer these as a starting point for discussion.

GOTTFREDSON & HIRSCHI’S SELF-CONTROL THEORY

In their tour de force, G&H (1990) challenged the field with a bold theory of criminal propensity rooted in classical theory assumptions of hedonistic rationality and universal (high) motivation to the immediate, simple pleasures from crime. Against their natural allure, G&H recognize that the benefits from crime are usually fleeting and minor and risk severe, albeit delayed, negative consequences. G&H thus theorize that after weighing costs and benefits, rational individuals should forgo crime. The question the theory addresses is: why do people offend given that “the balance of the total control structure favors conformity, even among offenders” (p.86)? In other words, how can a rational, hedonistic actor choose an objectively more painful act?2

The answer, according to SCT, is found in differences in time perspectives in cost consideration at the point of decision making. SCT is “a theory built on the idea that the decision to commit crime is governed by its short-term, immediate benefits, without consideration of long-term costs” (p.33). In SCT, people with low self-control remain rational and hedonistic, but are myopic; their criminal and analogous actions reflect nothing more than a lack of foresight or consideration of delayed consequences (Felson & Osgood 2008). According to G&H, people with high self-control consider the full consequences of their acts, recognize that crime risks more pain than gain, and thus are deterred by the delayed formal and informal consequences of crime. On the other hand, “people with low self-control [are] people inclined to follow momentary impulse without consideration of the long-term costs of such behavior” (p.191). Self-control is thus the key individual variable accounting for variation in crime and is defined as “the tendency of individuals to pursue short-term gratification without consideration of the long-term consequences of their acts” (p.177), making low self-control tantamount to criminality and providing “a theory of the ‘motivated’ offender” (Nagin & Paternoster 1993: 470).

Although G&H (1990) did not provide instructions for operationalizing their key concept (e.g., Akers 1991; Barlow 1991), a section entitled “The Elements of Self-Control” has been used as a guide for self-control measures (i.e., Grasmick et al. 1993). These elements or characteristics describe what G&H (1990: 89–91) theorize people who commit crime (and have low self-control) are like.3 In general, G&H state, “people who lack self-control will tend to be impulsive, insensitive, physical (as opposed to mental), risk-taking, shortsighted, and nonverbal, and they will tend therefore to engage in criminal and analogous acts” (p.90). Further, G&H assert that “there is considerable tendency for these traits to come together in the same people, and since the traits tend to persist through life, it seems reasonable to consider them as comprising a stable construct useful in the explanation of crime” (pp. 90–91).

Turning to the source of variation in self-control, G&H (1990: pp. 97–100) adopt a child-rearing model “for the origins of (failure to learn) self-control” (Hirschi 2004). Assuming that low self-control is the natural human condition (no special learning or motivation to crime needed), SCT proposes that effective parenting, which is driven by affection for the child and consists of monitoring, identification of misbehavior, and punishment of misbehavior, is sufficient to instill (high) self-control in children. Importantly, however, the critical period for the development of self-control is the first 8–10 years of life. After childhood, according to G&H, between-individual levels of self-control are fixed, and those who failed to develop self-control are doomed to “a moments pleasure for a life of misery” (Hirschi 2004:540). For those individuals fortunate to receive effective parenting, on the other hand, “socialization is a task that, once successfully accomplished, appears to be largely irreversible” (p.107). Thus, against competing theoretical causes of crime, which posit that changes in parenting, peers, or life circumstances post-childhood can influence criminality (e.g., Sampson & Laub 1993; Simons & Burt 2011), SCT asserts criminality is fixed at age 10, and that these putative social causes are in fact manifestations of self-control, and thus events without causal significance (p.119). In addition to stable criminality following childhood, SCT proposes versatility in crime and analogous acts, given that all crimes offer immediate pleasure at the expense of long-term costs.

RESEARCH ON SELF-CONTROL THEORY

Empirical assessments of SCT are plentiful, and at present, key theoretical postulates have all received sufficient empirical attention to assess their validity. Before discussing the key themes from this large body of research and their implications for the empirical validity of SCT, I should add a word about measurement. From its inception, there has been a debate around the operationalization of self-control (e.g., Akers 1991; Barlow 1991; Piquero 2008). As abovementioned, most criminological tests of SCT utilize personality/attitudinal measures of self-control operationalized based on “the elements of self-control,” including the most widely used measure, the Grasmick et al. (1993) scale and measures created in its image. Despite its predominance, several scholars (including Hirschi) have argued that this measure does not accurately capture G&H’s conception of self-control—of variability in people’s “consideration of the long-term consequences of their acts” (e.g., Burt 2012; Hirschi 2004; Marcus 2004).4 Given space constraints, the existence of several thorough discussions of the validity of common SCT measures, and, most importantly, the fact that I propose a shift away from SCT’s model of self-control, I forbear any further discussion of the conceptual validity of these measures. (Interested readers should consult these works for more information.)

Self-Control as Criminality

Turning to the central premise of SCT, that self-control is a primary cause of individual differences in offending, empirical support is copious. In particular, a wealth of research shows that low self-control is strongly associated with delinquency and street crimes, measured cross-sectionally and longitudinally, using both self-reports and official criminal records and controlling for criminal opportunity (for reviews see, Burt 2015; Hay & Meldrum 2016; Tittle et al. 2003). Meta-analyses covering the first and second decades of research on SCT report effect sizes of r>.20, after controlling for competing theoretical and relevant demographic variables, qualifying self-control as “one of the strongest known correlates of crime” (Pratt & Cullen 2000: 952; Vazsonyi et al 2017). Although surveys of youth engaged in minor and statistically normal offending predominate, self-control has been found to be significantly associated with crime among a variety of different populations (adult, adolescent, incarcerated, from various countries) with different outcomes (delinquency, crime imprudent behavior, violent offending convictions; see Tittle et al 2003; Vazsonyi et al. 2017). In total, the evidence clearly suggests a strong negative relationship between higher self-control and delinquency, street crimes, substance use, and analogous shortsighted behaviors.

Like other micro-level criminology theories, research assessing SCT is dominated by attitudinal self-report surveys and reports of past delinquency - much of it minor, inconsequential, and statistically normal. Moreover, we have very limited knowledge on the theorized effects of self-control in situ as a decision-making factor. Thus, the putative causal role of self-control as consideration of delayed (negative) consequences at the point of criminal decision-making is, as yet, largely untested.5 In sum, scholarship evinces that self-control is associated with crime, but when, how, and why is not particularly clear despite a copious amount of research on the topic. As I will discuss below, making headway on these issues—gaining a more complete and realistic picture of the role of self-control in crime—requires a more precise and multifaceted conception of self-control that recognizes motivations and restraints.

Development of Self-Control

Focusing on self-control development and consistent with SCT, research suggests that caregivers have a particularly strong influence on children’s self-control (e.g., Hope et al. 2004; Perrone et al. 2004). However, as Hay (2001) prominently noted in criminology, children’s levels of self-control are shaped not only by caregiver demandingness (as ‘effective parenting’) but also caregiver responsiveness in terms of warmth, support, and positive reinforcement (Baumrind 1966; Jacobsen et al. 1997). Subsequent research has demonstrated that children whose parents are both disciplining and supportive [a style Baumrind (1966) labeled authoritative] have higher self-control, all else equal, than those whose parents are authoritarian (disciplining but not supportive), permissive (supportive but not disciplining), or neglectful (e.g., Burt et al. 2006; Vazsonyi & Bellison 2007; Vazsonyi & Huang 2010).

Importantly, caregiving practices are not the only influence on self-control development. Instead, research evinces that neighborhood, peers, teachers, schools, and even wider social contexts shape levels, between-individual differences, and changes in self-control among children and adolescents (e.g., Burt et al. 2006; Meldrum 2008; Pratt et al. 2004; Simons & Burt; Teasdale & Silver 2009; Turner et al. 2005). In general, research suggests that traumatic, stressful life events, such as witnessing violence or experiencing criminal victimization, and adverse experiences, such as harsh or abusive parenting and racial discrimination, are associated with lower self-control (e.g., Agnew et al. 2011; Burt et al. 2012; 2017; Monahan et al. 2015; Sharkey 2012). Conversely, positive and supportive conditions, such as increased attachment to teachers and involvement with prosocial peers, is associated with higher self-control (e.g., Burt et al. 2006; Meldrum 2008). In sum, caregivers unequivocally have a - if not the - most significant influence on children’s self-control development during the formative years; however, other social-environmental cues and experiences that inculcate lessons about support, safety, and predictability as well as the wisdom of delaying gratification shape self-control.

Stability of Self-Control

Turning to SCT’s stability postulate, scholarship reveals plainly that self-control is not stable after childhood (8–10yo), but that within-individual levels and between-individual rankings of self-control continue to change through adolescence and into (at least early) adulthood (e.g., Hay & Forrest 2006; Na & Paternoster 2012; Ray et al. 2013; Winfree et al. 2006). For example, in their examination of self-control stability after SCT’s critical period of development, Burt et al. (2014) found that the proportion of reliable stability from wave 1 (10–12yo) to wave 6 (23–25yo) was a paltry .10. Studies using semi-parametric group-based trajectory models (SPGMs) to estimate similar developmental pathways suggest much greater (approximate) between-individual stability for many (~40–60%); however, these generally stable trajectories exist alongside trajectories with significant fluctuations, including individual losses, gains, and oscillations in levels of self-control (e.g., Burt et al. 2014; Hay & Forrest 2006; Ray et al. 2013). Moreover, SPGMs provide highly conservative estimates of between-individual stability as they ignore (considerable) within-group fluctuations in rankings. Importantly, evidence suggests that observed self-control changes are not due to measurement error and are non-trivial, in the sense that they are sizeable, consequential for crimes, and influenced by changes in the social factors just discussed (e.g., Burt et al. 2014; Hay et al. 2010; Na & Paternoster 2012). SCT’s stability proposition has been falsified, and self-control appears analogous to personality traits, which “are open systems that can be influenced by the environment at any age” (Roberts et al. 2008: 384).

To be sure, childhood appears to be an important or sensitive period for self-control development. Moreover, longitudinal studies suggest that some individuals do appear to have relatively stable low and high self-control, with the latter group bigger than the former. However, it is not clear how much of this stability and/or predictive ability is due to persistent heterogeneity versus state dependence (Nagin & Paternoster 1991; 2000), including the cumulative or cascading effects of low or high self-control (e.g., early self-control deficits beget academic, peer, and institutional difficulties that knife off opportunities for improving circumstances, e.g., Nagin & Paternoster 1994). There is no doubt that low self-control has numerous social consequences; however, they are not events without causal significance.

Social Consequences of Self-Control

Several studies have addressed SCT’s “social consequences of self-control proposition,” which postulates that events after age 10 are manifestations of self-control and only spuriously related to crime. To cut to the chase, the most accurate conclusion about self-control and social consequences is that the relationship is reciprocal (e.g., Evans et al. 1997; Thornberry et al. 1991; Wright et al. 1999). Through intertwined mechanisms of interactional and cumulative continuity, an individual’s self-control (along with numerous other factors and chance) influences socialization factors such as caregiving (e.g., “child effects” on parenting; Lytton 1990; Jang & Smith 1997; Scaramella et al. 2002) and selects or funnels individuals into different contexts and situations consistent with their preferences, which, in turn, sustains dispositions such as self-control (e.g., Evans et al. 1997; Simons et al. 2014; Wright et al. 1999). At the same time, self-control is but one of many factors that influences social situations and outcomes, and research shows that changes in social relationships, occupations, and other life alterations influence changes in self-control and crime across the life course (Burt et al. 2017; Sampson & Laub 1997).

For example, individuals with lower self-control, tend to select into more dangerous situations (e.g., stumbling home drunk late at night alone) and be criminally victimized (e.g., Schreck et al. 2006; see Pratt et al. 2013 for a meta-analysis), and crime victimization is associated with decreases in self-control (e.g., Agnew 2011; Monahan et al. 2015). Likewise, a copious and still growing body of research underscores the snowballing deleterious effects of justice system involvement on legitimate opportunity structures, especially educational and employment opportunities (e.g., Sampson & Laub 1997; Pager 2003), and relationships with family members (e.g., Stewart et al. 2002; Western 2018). Conversely, the successful exercise of self-control can result in positive reinforcements, including top marks as well as teacher and caregiver praise, and thereby provide a more supportive context for further enhancing self-control (Vohs & Ciarocco 2004). As with most things in human social life, early advantages tend to beget further advantages and disadvantages accumulate and can entrench individuals in difficult situations, even as the possibility for change remains ever present. More research on the circumstances, timing, and effects of individual differences in susceptibility to social changes is needed to better understand conditions most conducive to self-control change (for better or worse) as well as the factors that underlie observed stability, which includes social consequences.

Genetic Influences, Social Interventions, and Biological Maturation

Several scholars have challenged SCT’s child-rearing model arguing that self-control differences are largely a function of intergenerational transmission of genes rather than parenting practices (e.g., Beaver et al. 2007; Wright & Beaver 2005). Notably, G&H (1990) did not deny biological/genetic differences but rather argued that “[biological] individual differences may have an impact on the prospects for effective socialization (or adequate control). Effective socialization is, however, always possible whatever the configuration of individual traits” (p.96). Thus, SCT maintains that early developing biological or genetic differences may make parenting more difficult, but socialization differences are ultimately the major cause of differences in self-control.

The accumulated evidence over the past few decades suggests that G&H’s arguments about biological influences are, like other propositions, oversimplified. Some children appear to suffer from neurobiological deficits in response to genetic variation and/or environmental exposures (e.g., lead, nicotine) in early life that shape differences in executive functions, including working memory, attention shifting and concentration, episodic memory encoding, and learning contingencies that shape automaticity, among others (Gibson et al. 2006; Mullen et al. 2018; Winter & Sampson 2017). Moreover, to deny any genetic influences on the multitude of psychobiological factors that shape self-control processes, such as reward sensitivity and cognitive control (e.g., dopaminergic and serotonergic functioning; Carver et al. 2009; Chambers et al. 2004; DeYoung 2013), and punishment- or threat-sensitivity (e.g., amygdala reactivity) is indefensible (e.g., Forbes et al. 2009; Sapolsky 2016). That said, the evidence in support of the claim that parenting does not matter is flawed (e.g., Burt & Simons 2014), and the argument that “biogenic factors are largely responsible for the development of self-control” (Beaver et al. 2007) fails to acknowledge the “porousness of the biological to the social” (Meloni 2014: 6). In other words, counterposing a “biological” against a “socialization” model of self-control is misleading because socialization and life experiences affect development through biological mechanisms, including the wiring of our brain (e.g., Casey 2015). Thus, the idea that low self-control is a “brain-based disorder” (Delisi 2015), although implying (in this case, explicitly asserting) innateness and unchangeability actually says nothing about cause or fixity. (And, of course self-control processes are brain-based; where else would they be?)

Moreover, strong evidence contradicting genetic explanations and in support of social influences can be observed in studies of self-control interventions. Numerous studies using randomized, controlled designs demonstrate positive changes in self-control in response to interventions in childhood, especially school-based programs designed to enhance educational achievement (see Ursache et al. 2012) and to decrease conduct problems/delinquency (see Piquero et al. 2010; 2016). Although fewer in number, interventions to enhance parenting and/or self-control skills have also been found to effect significant and sustained change after age 10 (e.g., Brody et al. 2005).

Although, in my view, evidence convincingly debunks the claim that self-control differences are largely due to innate, genetic differences, this does not mean that biological factors are irrelevant. Not only do biological factors mediate the effects of social influences on enduring dispositions and behavioral outcomes, but recent scholarship in developmental neuroscience suggests that normative age-graded changes in neural circuitry may shed light on one of the brute facts of criminology: the age-crime curve, characteristic (at least) of Western industrialized (individualistic) countries (Hirschi & Gottfredson 1983; Steffensmeier et al. 2017). However, incorporating these advances requires moving beyond SCT’s singular focus on consideration of consequences to recognize variation in impulse strength and control motivation.

Brief Evaluation of SCT’s Assumptions and Measurement

Unequal Motivation

SCT, like all theories, is grounded on assumptions, several of which have come under direct empirical scrutiny. Focusing first on motivation, the evidence is clear that SCT’s assumption of invariant motivation to (or temptation by pleasures from) crime is untenable (Tittle & Botchkovar 2005). Instead, individual differences in preferences (e.g., sensation seeking; tastes; desires), normative beliefs and personal standards, constraints and perceived costs, goals, opportunities, peer reinforcements, and other factors influence variation in the general motivation to commit crimes, the perceived pleasures and gains from different types of crimes, and the situational incentives to crime (e.g., Burt & Simons 2013; Jacobs & Wright 1999; Nagin & Paternoster 1993; Tittle et al. 2004). We are not all equally tempted by opportunities to use LSD, to shoplift, to vandalize, and to exact violent revenge on someone who harmed our reputation (Felson & Osgood 2008; Geis 2000). Motivation to crime is a variable; it varies across context, situations, and between individuals for crimes and different types of crime, and it influences offending in part by influencing the effects of self-control on offending (e.g., Burt & Simons 2013; Tittle & Botchkovar 2005).

Calculated Crime

Research also suggests that G&H’s assertions about the nature of crime - as unplanned, impulsive acts in the face of opportunity with little loss and less gain - do not accurately reflect the reality of crime as we know it. Certainly many street crimes and perhaps most delinquency result from individuals’ failing to recognize and weigh delayed consequences against meager gains in decision-making. However, as several scholars have articulated, some crimes, especially elite white collar and corporate crimes, involve careful deliberation and a prioritization of long-term gains, which can be relatively substantial (Benson & Moore 1992; Geis 2000; Simpson 2013). Furthermore, not just elite but also street crimes, such as drug dealing, can be lucrative endeavors, especially when compared to the available alternatives (e.g., Fagan & Freeman 1999; McCarthy & Hagan 2001; Levitt & Venkatesh 2000). As Bandura (1996: 20) noted “It takes proficient [self-control] to be a successful burglar, terrorist, unscrupulous businessperson, or corrupt politician”

Even seemingly non-utilitarian street violence can, in some circumstances, be motivated by long-term considerations. While much street violence tends to erupt from what might be seen as minor disputes, some of this seemingly impulsive, senseless violence is driven by a desire for respect -- a currency providing status and safety in the future (e.g., Anderson 1990; 1999).6 To be sure, the point of these examples is not to suggest that many crimes do not result from impulsive decision making, but rather it is to show that the assumption that consideration of long-term costs leads invariably to non-crime because crime risks more pain than gain cannot be reconciled with the reality of crime. It is empirically invalid to assume that crimes result from failing to consider the long-term consequences or that crime cannot be utility maximizing in the streets or the suites. SCT’s overriding assumption that conformity maximizes utility and crime does not pay reflects a middle-class bias that ignores the windfalls from elite crimes and the limited opportunity structures facing the truly disadvantaged.

Situational Variability, Domain Specificity, and Irrationality

SCT presumes a domain-general self-control ability (e.g., Muraven et al. 2006, Tittle et al. 2004). Although situations may vary in their opportunities for immediate gratification, SCT assumes a relatively constant temporal extension in the future in cost-benefit calculations shaping action choices that maximize utility. In this view, an individual with relatively high self-control who cognitively extends his consequence consideration beyond the immediate situation well into the future, would carry this tendency with him to school, to work, out on the weekend, when making decisions about dessert, and when to go to bed; he makes rational choices based on the calculations from the relatively temporally extended consequences he considers. Given the highly conditional nature of human behavior, along with the reality that different situational contingencies influence even such fundamental deliberative processes as how much time we have to consider consequences (e.g., deciding whether to run the light just turning red when running late to work versus considering for several days whether to report your extra income on your taxes), this casting of self-control as an unconditional general capacity, with situation-specific variance as merely noise or error, is oversimplified. Research suggests that in many choice situations relevant to crime, people are not acting with a situationally stable steady reservoir of (trait) self-control. Instead, situational precursors (e.g., prior exertions of self-control, general fatigue) and characteristics (e.g., emotional situations; alcohol involvement) nullify (or moderate) the influence of self-control capacity or ability, producing variation in state self-control (e.g., Muraven et al. 1998, 1999; Baumeister et al. 2018).

Future criminological research might beneficially focus on these situational departures from a person’s general self-control capacity to better illuminate when self-control matters for crime and how situational variations in psychological processes underlie domain-specific outcomes in interaction with individual dispositions. One potentially fruitful approach involves identifying situation-behavior profiles in decision-making and impulsive action (e.g., Mischel et al. 2002, Tsukayama et al. 2013).7 Focusing on alcohol use, for example, most people are perhaps all too well aware of the fact that inebriation influences normal decision-making processes, and we know that much crime, including serious violence, occurs under the influence of alcohol (e.g., Jacobs & Wright 1999, Pridemore 2004). Steele & Joseph’s (1990) well-known and empirically supported alcohol myopia model explicates how the state of inebriation alters people’s normal decision-making and self-control capacities. Specifically, this work shows that acute alcohol consumption narrows attention to salient situational cues, impairs cognitive processing and cost considerations, and therefore increases disinhibition and self-control failures (for an excellent review, see Giancola et al. 2010). Another example of a situation-behavior profiles (as patterned decrements in self-control) is found research on emotional duress. Ample research suggests that people’s self-control abilities deteriorate when under high-arousal emotional distress and in emotionally charged situations (e.g., Leith & Baumeister 1996, Tice et al. 2001).8 Emotionally charged situations seem to alter or hijack our normal (cool) reasoning capacities (e.g., Kahneman 2011, Mischel et al. 1973). Among other influences, emotional distress can decrease individuals’ typical self-control by promoting a short-term prioritization of affect regulation over long-term goals (i.e., If you feel bad, do it!) (Tice et al. 2001).

Normal reasoning processes facilitating self-control are also altered in response to immediate threats. Specifically, cues of threat can bypass our normal deliberative (primarily prefrontal cortex (PFC)) neurocircuits to facilitate immediate, defensive reactions through a rapid-response pathway (Miller & Cohen 2001). Bypassing these deliberative circuits facilitates immediate, unthinking survival responses. For example, when an out-of-control car is veering toward the sidewalk, one can push a child and dive out of the way before even cognizing the situation. However, this response speed comes at the expense of accuracy, producing circumstances where we may rapidly respond to a perceived threat (a gun) and respond defensively (shoot the person multiple times) before engaging our more accurate, deliberative neurocircuitry, which recognizes that the gun was actually a cell phone (Sapolsky 2017). In these situations, which fortunately appear to be rare for many of us, action as rapid response occurs without any rational cost-benefit calculations at all.

As these examples illustrate, when people drink heavily, are under extreme and chronic distress, and or perceiving serious threat—all situations when irrational and/or shortsighted behaviors disproportionately occur—they are often operating without the full use of their self-control facilities. Compounding the problem, evidence suggests that repeating these situation-response patterns over time contributes to lower levels of self-control, perhaps especially in adolescence, as the underlying neurobiological pathways are potentiated (Casey 2015, Quinn et al. 2011). In sum, SCT’s assumption of a domain-general, situationally invariant level of self-control that influences cost-benefit calculations and rational choices as an unconditional generality does not capture the complex reality of human behavior in response to situational contingencies. Ample evidence that such departures from deliberative decision-making processes are involved in a nontrivial amount of criminal behavior behooves us to move away from the SCT model of domain generality to focus on understanding these situation-behavior profiles and connecting them to situational self-control processes.

The Elements Measures of Self-Control: Recasting as a Criminality Personality Typology

Amid the ongoing measurement debate, several studies have assessed the unidimensionality of the “elements of self-control” in the Grasmick et al. (1993) scale (and similar ones). Although not unequivocal, psychometric analyses suggest that the elements of self-control are multidimensional, such that they do not come together in the same people to compose a latent personality trait (e.g., Arneklev et al. 1993, Burt & Simons 2013, Longshore et al. 1996). Research has also shown that these various elements of self-control differentially predict crime and different types of crime (e.g., Arneklev et al. 1993, Greenberg et al. 2002, Pfefferbaum & Wood 1994).

These findings are consistent with scholarship in personality psychology showing that these elements are associated with different (and independent) facets of personality (e.g., Marcus 2004, Morizot & Le Blanc 2005, Romero et al. 2003). The unidimensionality of the elements of self-control is also contradicted by research in developmental psychology and neuroscience. For example, a growing body of scholarship suggests that impulsivity, as a tendency to act without thinking (Pickering & Gray 1999), is distinct from risk or sensation seeking, as a preference for intense or novel stimuli (Forbes & Dahl 2010, Zuckerman 1994), and these distinct personality traits have distinct neurobiological underpinnings and developmental timetables (Casey 2015, Ernst et al. 2006, Steinberg et al. 2008).

Thus, a wealth of evidence suggests that treating the elements of self-control as a unidimensional latent construct is unwise (Burt et al. 2014). To be sure, this constellation of elements is currently one of the strongest correlates of crime, and thus may be usefully considered a criminality personality typology, but one that is neither consistent with SCT assumptions nor reflective of G&H’s conceptual definition of self-control (Burt et al. 2014, Hirschi 2004, Marcus 2004). Moving forward, a recognition that this elemental self-control personality does not reflect self-control, in general or as conceptualized in SCT, is in order.

MOVING FORWARD: RECONCEPTUALIZING SELF-CONTROL QUA SELF-CONTROL

The concept of self-control has a long history; one that is generally overlooked in criminology. Dating back to Socrates, Plato, and Aristotle, and later Freud (1933/1948) and James (1890/1950), renowned scholars have been intrigued by humans’ sophisticated abilities to delay gratification in the service of abstract long-term goals. Contemporary scholarship, especially in psychology, sparked by Mischel and colleagues’ famous marshmallow experiments from the 1960s (e.g., Bandura & Mischel 1965, Mischel & Ebbesen 1970, Mischel & Baker 1975), has elucidated the different psychological processes underlying self-control (Baumeister et al. 1998, 2007; Carver & Scheier 1981; Kotabe & Hofmann 2015; Rachlin 2000). Until recently, this work focused largely on volitional control processes; however, more recent scholarship incorporates empirically evident variability in motivational processes, including individual variation in impulse strength (e.g., Duckworth & Steinberg 2015, Hofmann et al. 2009, Steinberg et al. 2008). Yet this large and growing collection of facts and insights from psychology, bolstered by mechanistic insights from neuroscience, has remained on the periphery of criminological work on self-control, which usually starts and stops with G&H’s SCT.

As noted, the most significant challenge to integrating this scholarship into criminology is conceptual inconsistency. What G&H define as self-control is not consistent with its colloquial usage or common scientific definition (Duckworth et al. 2019, Rachlin 1974, Vohs & Baumeister 2004). With a few exceptions (Burt & Simons 2013, Wikström & Trieber 2007), criminologists have largely overlooked self-control’s “jingle-jangle problem,”9 treating differences between self-control, impulsivity, ego control, self-regulation, delay discounting, willpower, risk seeking, executive functions, and reward seeking, among others, as largely superfluous (e.g., Hay & Meldrum 2016, Moffitt et al. 2011). The concept of self-control has actually been lost in the exuberance around SCT. Here, I build on recent scholarship decomposing self-control processes to clearly define self-control and differentiate between these concepts. I do so fully recognizing that residual differences in definitions remain, and yet a general consensus across a broad literature exists (e.g., Duckworth et al. 2019).

After covering definitional issues, I illustrate how these psychological processes shape differences in self-control and discuss their added value in understanding crime. In particular, this refined conceptualization allows for the consideration of the interplay between impulse strength and control efforts as dueling influences on criminal decision-making. This conceptualization also facilitates criminological recognition of several themes of recent researching, including that self-controlled behavior is distinct from the underlying psychological processes, and that failure to consider competing psychological processes (dueling impulsigenic and control/volitional processes) can lead to faulty conclusions about the causes of self-control failures (e.g., Duckworth & Steinberg 2015, Kotabe & Hofmann 2015). Aptly describing these competing forces, Sir T.S. Clouston remarked, “The driver may be so weak that he cannot control well-broken horses, or the horses may be so hard-mouthed that no driver can pull them up” (cited by James 1890, p. 540). Finally, I zoom out and discuss challenges and fruitful lines of research on self-control, emphasizing the fact that although understanding intrapsychic mechanisms is important, self-control does not operate in a social vacuum. Instead, social structural, contextual, interactional, and individual factors function as codeterminants of self-control outcomes (Bandura 1996, Mischel & Ayduk 2004, Simons & Burt 2011). I conclude by identifying several underexplored avenues for future research on self-control and crime.

Defining Self-Control

Self-control is variously considered an act, a capacity, and a depletable resource, but uniting these different usages is the key idea of resisting a desire or hedonic impulse - momentarily perceived as more gratifying - in the service of more valuable long-term goals and standards (Duckworth et al. 2019; Duckworth & Steinberg 2015; Kotabe & Hoffman 2015). Self-control—a subset of self-regulation—is the effortful inhibition of prepotent, immediately rewarding responses following a choice situation known as a self-control conflict. The conflict is the intrapsychic recognition of the incompatibility of a lower-order situational goal as a desire for immediate pleasure (a second piece of cake, going to a rave instead of studying for a test) and a more highly valued long-term goal (health/slimness; good grades/successful career/no criminal record). This presumes that higher-order goals - as relatively “cool” cognitive representations of desired end states, rewards, or achievements - are organized hierarchically (Powers 1973; Krugslanski et al. 2002).10

Despite goal hierarchies, intrapsychic conflict arises because of the temporal aspect, namely the universal tendency among humans (and other animals) to display steep temporal discounting of rewards and punishments (Ainslie 1974; Rachlin 1974; Logue 1988). Life is uncertain and unpredictable, and tomorrow is not assured, so to a degree that differs among individuals and situations, we discount later rewards and punishments (Chisholm 1999; Mischel et al. 1989). Because we discount rewards and punishments, lower-order goals spike in relative value when they are in the immediate present as higher-order goals remain in the distant future (Ainslie 2001). Self-control conflict is thus this particular choice dilemma defined by a clash of situational impulses for smaller, sooner rewards against larger, later rewards or delayed punishments.11 As Rachlin (1974) averred: “Take the temporal issue away and the issue of self-control goes away as well. If the unpleasantness and pleasantness are completely contemporaneous, self-control is not involved.” Recent research suggests that trait self-control is associated with how well people deal with self-control conflicts but not with how they deal with other motivational conflict suggesting that self-control conflict is a unique dilemma (Hofmann et al. 2014), and this distinction is further supported by neuroimaging research (e.g., Sommerville et al. 2010; Volkow et al. 2010).

Thus, as an act, self-control (also called willpower, effortful inhibition, or ego control) is the behavioral forgoing of immediate gratification, the effortful restraining of oneself against immediate temptation in the service of more enduringly valued goals (Duckworth & Steinberg 2015; Kotabe & Hofmann 2015). Self-control failures are unsuccessful efforts to forgo temptation. As a general capacity, self-control is the overall ability or tendency of individuals to resist immediate temptations for long-term goals, all else equal, much like the weight and reps a person can usually bench press.12 Finally, as a depletable resource, as conceptualized in the strength model which highlights situational variability, self-control is conceived as akin to a limited-capacity muscle, which is fatigued and can experience failure with repeated short-term exertions but can also be strengthened over time with moderate practice and sufficient recovery (Baumeister et al. 1998; Muraven et al. 2006).

Perhaps quite obviously, SCT’s conceptualization of self-control as “consideration of future consequences” is not tantamount to this general conception of self-control (Wikström & Trieber 2007; Burt & Simons 2013), and these differences are not superfluous.13 Self-control may be exerted in response to recognizing more highly-valued goals or long-term costs, but recognizing costs is a necessary but insufficient step in the self-control process. Acting on impulse without consideration or deliberation can be a failure of self-control resulting from lack of deliberation, or the act may not be problematic, (i.e., if the act does not violate the individual’s standards or conflict with long-term goals). If there is no impulse-goal conflict, then self-control is not involved. Thus, self-control failure cannot be inferred from criminal or otherwise risky or unhealthy behavior. If Harvey believes that as a star maker he deserves sexual access to his actress employees, such that his frequent sexual coercion involves no goal conflict (or concern that he will get caught), his “bad” behavior does not involve a self-control failure. Assuming self-control failure from behavior, as SCT does, is thus misguided. Furthermore, incompatibility of immediate desires and long-term costs triggers a self-control conflict but does not, unfortunately, inevitably lead to success. For example, Donald, who swore off shoplifting having almost been caught a few weeks ago, visits Target and immediately experiences the urge to steal something after entering the store. After wandering the store for 20 minutes deliberating - considering consequences and reminding himself of his promise to not steal anymore—he then steals the item anyway. Is this a self-control failure? Yes.14 Like Donald, all of us sometimes fail to resist our temptations.

Thus, the construct G&H label high “self-control” is better labeled “consideration of future consequences” with low self-control better conceptualized15 as impulsivity (acting without thinking in response to situational stimuli).16 As a necessary step to align criminological scholarship with knowledge advances on the multifaceted psychological processes underlying self-control, I propose relabeling SCT’s low self-control as “impulsivity” (or even “shortsightedness,” see van Geldner et al. 2017). From this point forward, my use of self-control refers to the standard definition of self-control, as the effortful control of the self against temptation in the service of enduring goals, not G&H’s self-control.

Control Motivation, Control Capacity, and Dual-Influence Models

The experience of temptation in self-control conflicts can be described as an intrapsychic tug-of-war between impulses and control motivations. Self-control outcomes thus result from the interplay of these processes, and a joint consideration between these impelling and restraining forces will enhance models of self-control (Strack & Deutsch 2004; Hofmann et al. 2009). Consistent with recent duel-influence models (Hoffmann et al. 2009; 2012; Steinberg et al. 2008,), I define an impulse as a psychological urge or prepotent response tendency, which arises when global motivations, rooted in innate or learned associations and reinforcements (e.g., food, safety, status, rest, alcohol/drugs, sex), meet specific activating stimuli in the environment (Hofmann et al. 2009). Although varying in strength, impulses generally possess strong incentive value, which motivates approach to immediately rewarding stimuli and away from threat (Lowenstein 1996; Metcalfe & Mischel 1999). Impulses are immediate in both a temporal and a spatial sense, and people can experience impulses without consciously knowing why (Ainslie 1975; Hofmann et al. 2009). Impulses emerge in a relatively automatic manner as subcortical reward processing centers (e.g., nucleus accumbens) respond to situational stimuli and appraisals against the backdrop of internal need states and learning history (Hofmann & Kotabe 2012; Hoffman & Van Dillen 2012). “Following our impulses would be biologically adaptive if were designed to live only for today and without concern for other people’s well-being” (Hofmann et al 2009:163). Just as people differ in their control capacities, they also differ in their reactions to various stimuli. These differences are likely due to a combination of current need states, biological endowment, and differences in learning history (e.g., Hofmann et al. 2009; Strack & Deutsch 2004).

In contrast to impulses, higher-order goals are usually more abstract (imagined futures) and more strongly associated with one’s values and virtues (i.e. I am not going to use drugs because they are unhealthy and illegal; Fujita 2011; Kotabe & Hofmann 2015). Like impulses, higher-order goals vary in strength. At a cognitive level, higher order goal strength may correspond with the accessibility of the associated target end state and supporting mental representations (Fishbach & Ferguson 2007), and, in general, goal-strength is determined by at least three (often correlated) factors: importance (the degree to which a goal represents a high-priority objective; Fishbach et al. 2003), commitment, one’s determination to achieve the goal (Klein et al. 1999; Hirschi 1969), and self-efficacy and perceived control, one’s perceived ability to successfully achieve the act and avoid obstacles (Bandura 1977; Mischel & Ayduk 2004). Higher-order goal strength, in concert with situational factors, shapes control motivation17, defined as the aspiration to control temptation (see Carver & Sheier 1981; Kotabe & Hofmann 2015; Kruglanski et al. 2002).

Recent models of self-control have recognized the importance of monitoring and detection of response conflicts as key processes in the activation of effortful self-control and which vary between individuals, across situations, and contingent on goal and impulse strengths (e.g., Inzlicht & Legault 2014; Milkman et al. 2008; Kotabe & Hofmann 2015). Conflict-monitoring theory, supported by several experiments and simulations, proposes the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activates in response to the detection of conflict, functioning as an internal alarm that triggers self-control processes (Botvinick et al. 2001; Yeung et al. 2004). When self-control processes are activated by impulse-goal conflict, the impulse becomes a temptation, and the higher-order goal becomes a self-control goal (Kotabe & Hofmann 2015). Whether self-control is successful depends on whether the control effort expended is sufficient to overcome the temptation, with control effort defined as the actual amount of mental energy a person invests to overcome inhibition and work toward higher-order goals (Kotabe & Hofmann 2015; Muraven et al. 2006). Individuals do not usually expend maximum self-control effort in response to every self-control conflict (e.g., Baumeister et al. 2017). Thus, understanding the factors shaping motivation, effort, impulse strength, and various factors that moderate control efforts (e.g., responding to repeated peer pressure to violate the law; inhibiting aggressive responding to frequent bullying) is necessary because one’s maximum self-control capacity is not sufficient to explain variation in behavior, even all else equal (which it never is).

Longstanding, but recently elaborated dual-influence models organize these oppositional forces into dueling impulsigenic (shaping impulse/temptation strength) and volitional control processes (particularly executive functions18; Hofmann et al. 2009; Metcalfe & Mischel 1999; Steinberg et al. 2008). This model proposes that variation in both impulsigenic (e.g., sensation seeking, reward sensitivity, cravings) and control processes vary across time, individuals, and situations, and both need to be taken into account. Impulse strength is pitted against control effort, which is jointly determined by control motivation and control capacity. Although some elaborations of dual-influence models were understood to imply discrete control versus impulse brain substrates, this framework is functional not neuroanatomical (Duckwork & Steinberg 2015). Rather than discrete dual “systems”, more recent work, informed by advances in neuroscience, adopts circuit-based rather than region-based explanations. Circuit-based accounts highlight differences between individuals and changes across time in the wiring and fine-tuning of connections within and between complex subcortical and cortical prefrontal and limbic circuitries, which shapes variation in impulse strength and cognitive control (see Casey 2015 for an excellent review).

Psychological and developmental neuroscientific advances have greatly enhanced our understanding of multifaceted factors involved in self-control. Criminologists have the distinct challenge of considering how these factors shape criminal behavior. To this task, I now turn.

The Complex Self-Control Process: A Heuristic Overview

Figure 1 displays a simplified overview of a self-control conflict relevant to crime. Here I incorporate insights from several different models of self-control to highlight individual differences and complex multistage variation, which SCT ignores (Baumeister & Vohs 2004; Duckworth et al. 2019; Kotabe & Hoffman 2015; Tittle et al. 2003). Notably, the process depicted in this figure is not intended as a theory of self-control, as it contains no predictions, but merely a heuristic model of self-control processes, oversimplified, but useful for illustrating several key points about self-control. For clarity, these stages are presented sequentially, but in reality, these overlap in a dynamic, reciprocal process. Although I spotlight sources of within- and between-individual variation in self-control processes in Figure 1, it should be remembered that these always operate in interaction with ongoing cultural and situational influences.

Which of the following is the source of low self-control in the general theory of crime?

Self-Control: Heuristic Overview of the Processes Involved in Self-Control Situations.

As shown, self-control consists of several intersecting social cognitive (qua perceptual), motivational, and affective processes shaped by past experiences, sociocultural context, precursors and situational elements, among others (Kotabe & Hofmann 2015).19 To any given situation, people bring a unique set of social schemas, memories, beliefs, and interests, which shape, inter alia, perceptions and definitions of the situation, perceptions of opportunities, and cost-benefit appraisals. These individual differences explain variable responses to what appears to be, to the outside observer, the same objective situation (Duckworth et al. 2019; Simons & Burt 2011). These percepts and appraisals influence the interpretation of and reaction to stimuli (e.g., shaping desire and strength of impulse) and representations of costs and rewards; examples include, hostile views of relationships and internal standards, beliefs, or morality. For example, Wikström’s (2006; Wikström & Trieber 2007) situational action theory emphasizes the importance of morality or personal standards in shaping perceptions of opportunities for crime. They theorized that many people in most situations never even perceive or cognitively encode opportunities for crime; thus, crime never becomes a temptation that they have to resist. Similarly, Simons and Burt (2011) highlighted the importance of social schemas in shaping situational definitions relevant to temptations and action alternatives, theorizing that patterned variation in social experiences profoundly shaped by social position, influence how people perceive the world and their action alternatives. Motivational aspects include goals, desires, preferences (likes and dislikes), reward sensitivity, pain/loss/risk aversion, interests, values, and commitments, among others that are also clearly shaped by social position, past experiences, and affective factors. These affective aspects include more stable characteristics such as optimism versus pessimism, hope versus despair, and more transient or situational emotions such as anger, fear, sadness, joy, and biological (hunger, pain) states. These various individual difference factors are all intertwined in an unfolding self-control situation alongside numerous situational contingencies.

As the foregoing suggests, SCT oversimplifies a more complicated, nuanced process. The most important takeaway from Figure 1 is the range of salient individual differences that shape impulses or temptations and control motivations and that are wrongly assumed to be constant or unimportant in SCT (Burt & Simons 2013; Tittle et al. 2004). What G&H astutely note about the complexities of childrearing is also true about self-control processes: “what may at first appear to be unproblematic turns out problematic indeed. Many things can go wrong.” (p.98). First, as noted, SCT’s theorized causal variable of ‘consideration of consequences’ captures but one source of variation at one stage (#2) in this process. And, variation in this stage is also influenced by situational factors as well as individual factors such as self-monitoring, time orientation, and the ability to recall the lessons from past experiences and apply that information moving forward (e.g., Paternoster & Pogarsky 2009).

SCT overlooks important variation in preceding factors, depicted in stage 1, many of which are the focus of competing theories (e.g., Akers, Sutherland, Wikström & Trieber 2007). Ample research inside and outside of criminology underscores the importance of individual difference in shaping perception of and temptation by opportunities for crime. For example, individual differences shaping variation in these stage 1 processes (variation in the perception of opportunities for immediate gratification and the degree to which one is tempted by these opportunities), include social schemas that shape situational definitions (i.e. hostile views); personal standards, values, morality; preferences; habits; to name a few. As Wikstrom and Trieber (2007) noted, many of us never perceive opportunities for crimes that exist all around us; we are never tempted; and we do not experience a self-control conflict and thus have to expend self-control. Similarly, what is perceived as a serious threat or affront in one person’s interpretation is an accident or a mistake by another, and these definitions shape what might follow from an action (violent retaliation, apology, tolerance; Simons & Burt 2011) producing behavioral differences relevant to crime potentially having nothing to do with self-control. Again, if there is no higher-order goal or standard that conflicts with an impulse there is no impulse-goal conflict and self-control is irrelevant to the action.

Briefly discussed above, a growing body of research emphasizes the importance of desire or temptation strength (see Hoffmann et al. 2009; Rawn & Vohs 2011), and the degree to which such temptations are domain-specific and affected by personal standards (e.g., Tsukayama et al. 2012; 2013; Wikström & Trieber 2007). Although we are all tempted by something, we are not all equally tempted by crimes, and our preferences—shaped by past experiences, social position, culture, context, sex/gender, and biological factors—all influence the nature and strength of our temptations. Research on domain-specificity in impulses and in exposure to everyday temptations highlights the effects of individual personality differences, such as sensation seeking, on the nature, strength, and experience of temptations as well as the interaction between desire strength and resistance in determining behavioral enactment (e.g., Hoffmann et al. 2012; Imhoff et al. 2013; Tsukayama et al. 2013). Research also reveals the potentially surprising finding that, in general, people who experience fewer self-control failures do so not because their self-control efforts are more successful, but rather because they experience fewer temptations. That is, in their everyday lives people with high self-control are less reliant on “in the moment” cognitive control of impulses as effortful exertion of self-control (Galla & Duckworth 2015). Instead, the everyday lives of these individuals are more characterized by beneficial habits. When people act out of habit, they respond automatically to contextual cues or stimuli, and thus do not consider action alternative, engage in cost considerations, or have to expend effort inhibiting unwanted responses (Galla & Duckworth 2015; Ouellette & Wood 1998; Wikström 2006).

Moreover, several paths that SCT assumes are invariant (noted in italics in the figure) have been shown to be variable. Specifically, a consideration of consequences does not inevitably lead to a recognition of conflict with long-term goals. Thinking about the future, and recognizing that one’s contemplated act in the present threatens this distant future (recognizing impulse-goal conflict) are two different things, and various emotional and need states, including joy, anger, and despair, influence the connections we make in our cognitions and calculations, which is how the same person in the same situation can forgo the offer of cocaine one night and use it the next. Similarly, recognizing that an act is not utility maximizing, and deciding that one should forgo the act (and exert self-control), does not automatically lead to successful self-control. Unfortunately, we all “fail” at self-control sometimes, whether it is our commitment to getting up early to exercise, our decision to have only one drink, to report all our income to the IRS, to drive the speed limit, etc. Whether a response to peer pressure, fatigue, lack of success or frustration, or changing affect, or - as was often the case in Mischel’s marshmallow experiments—the desire or temptation for the object of immediate gratification is just too much to resist, we fail despite our initial decisions to exert self-control. In short, although consideration of consequences is a prerequisite for self-control it is, unfortunately for us, not sufficient to ensure success in controlling temptations.

Future Research: Ordering the Complexity

SCT is gloriously parsimonious, but empirically invalid. The solution, however, does not require drastic shift into a theoretical trap of overwhelming complexity, as the above discussion might suggest. Instead, in my view, a pressing agenda for research on self-control and crime is identifying for whom, when, and how self-control failures produce crime along with a concerted effort to understand the root causes of individual differences in underlying cognitive control and motivational processes. Given evidence of malleability to social influences into emerging adulthood, this work should focus on identifying social influences that are amenable to change through social policies and interventions. Below I identify two lines of self-control research in criminology that may be fruitful.

Adolescent Vulnerability and Opportunity

Over the past decade, a new perspective on adolescent risk-taking and decision making, informed by advances in developmental neuroscience, has emerged (e.g., Casey et al. 2008; Steinberg 2008). This model departs from the assumption that impulses remain constant over development and that what changes with maturation is cognitive control abilities (Ernst et al 2006; Duckworth & Steinberg 2015; Steinberg et al. 2008). This position has proven particularly useful in illuminating changes in risk-taking in adolescence, providing new insights into the age-crime curve (e.g., Steinberg et al. 2008). If self-control were merely dependent on volitional control capacities, then it would be difficult to explain the increased real-world and laboratory-based risky behavior observed from childhood to adolescence, especially given evidence of gradually maturing cognitive control abilities into the mid-twenties (Giedd 2008; Paus 2005; Steinberg 2008). Based on findings from developmental human imaging and nonhuman animal studies, recent “imbalance models” emphasize the impulsigenic role of adolescent heightened mesolimbic responsiveness to rewards against insufficiently matured cognitive control circuitry (Casey 2015). The peak in adolescent risk taking thus appears to be, at least in part, a function of increases in reward sensitivity, which precede the structural maturation of more robust cognitive control circuitry (Steinberg et al. 2008), leading some scholars to liken this period of susceptibility to strong temptations to one of “all gasoline, no brakes, and no steering wheel” (Bell & McBride 2010: 565).

Although catchy, this “all gas, no brakes, and no steering wheel” metaphor for adolescence is oversimplified (Casey 2015). Instead of a general mesolimbic sensitivity to rewards, adolescents show an increase in sensitivity to certain incentives, particularly status-related rewards (money, peer acceptance), and novelty seeking (Galvan et al. 2006; Steinberg et al. 2008). For example, Chein et al.’s (2011) driving simulations showed that when operating solo in the service of time-related rewards, adolescents are no more likely to than adults to run a red light (in the computer task). However, in the presence of peers, adolescents become significantly more likely to run the red light, whereas adults’ decisions were unchanged (also Gardner & Steinberg 2005). Moreover, this different pattern of risk taking by peer context was associated with differential anticipatory activation of reward circuits in adolescents.20 These lab-based driving simulation results are consonant with real-life behavior; adolescents, unlike adults, are more likely to get in car accidents in the presence of peers (Chen et al 2000). In short, a growing number of studies suggest that adolescence is a time of heightened sensitivity to certain incentives (money, peer acceptance) and in some contexts (in the presence of peers), and this heightened sensitivity appears to be undergirded by changes in dopamine-rich regions of the brain (i.e. ventral striatum) that are important for learning and predicting action outcomes (e.g., Jones et al. 2011, Lin et al. 2012, Meshi et al. 2013, Rademacher et al. 2010). Moreover, not only do adolescents show increased responsivity to particular positive reward cues that may tax the individuals’ control capacities, but also adolescents, especially males, display heightened amygdala reactivity to cues of potential threat and increased impulsive responding (relative to adults or children; Dreyfuss et al. 2014; Hare et al. 2008). Thus, adolescents’ heightened reactivity to both positive and negative socio-environmental cues, relative to children and adults, along with limited relative control capacity to regulate these responses makes adolescence a window of vulnerability to temptation (Casey 2015). Future criminological research would benefit from building on dual-influence adolescence imbalance models to enhance knowledge on the role of adolescent heightened impulses, especially in social contexts, on criminal behavior in concert with the many social challenges during this period, and their interplay with culture and individual differences (e.g., Burt et al. 2014; Steffensmeier et al. 2017).

However, as Steinberg (2008) and others have noted, adolescence is also a period of opportunity for positive change in self-control capacities (Dahl 2004). The extensive remodeling in reward and control neurocircuitry—ranging from neurogenesis to programmed cells death, synaptic pruning, and myelination (e.g., Casey 2015; Steinberg 2008)—opens the adolescent brain to social-environmental influence creating a sensitive period for learning and developmental change (Anderson 2003; Blakemore & Mills 2014; Ellis et al. 2012). This period of heightened plasticity provides an opportunity for adaptive change in response to environmental, especially social, cues about not only danger, support, and resource availability, but also individuals’ social status, attractiveness, and ability to amass resources (Jackson & Ellis 2009). Evidence that adolescence and young adulthood are crucial periods for personality change underscores the import of investigating the causal mechanisms responsible for personality change over these periods in the life course. Because adolescence is characterized by significant biological and social changes, it will be important for research to disentangle the effects of social changes from neurobiological maturation while attending to their interactions. Future work might explore sex/gender differences. At present, there is a dearth of knowledge on how gender dynamics, as main effects or in interaction with biological ones (such as hormones), influence the development of these self-control processes. Moreover, given well-known sex/gender differences in impulsivity, risk-taking, and crime (e.g., Cross et al. 2011; Simons & Burt, 2011), how such biological factors act in concert with social influences to shape differences in various distinct self-control processes between males and females is needed.

Self-Control in Context: Hope, Self-Efficacy, and Control Motivation

In this paper, I have proffered a view of self-control as an emergent interactional process shaped by much more than consideration of consequences. A move away from SCT’s singular focus on within-the-person control will allow us to better understand patterned variation in self-control processes and criminal outcomes across context, social position, and development and their influences on impulse strength, control motivation, and control effort. Moving forward, increased attention to the influence of social factors and their effects on and interplay with individual differences in shaping self-control processes related to crime could be especially valuable.

For example, investigations of the cumulative and interactional effects of social disadvantages on self-control processes might profitably build on recent scholarship in developmental evolutionary psychology as well as criminological work revealing the profound lack of hope and despair among many youths in highly disadvantaged positions (e.g., Anderson 1999; Brezina et al. 2009). The effects of poverty, rampant crime, racism, in part through their effects on soft skills (including self-control skills) greatly impede performance in educational realms (e.g., Burt et al. 2017; Heckman 2008; Vohs 2013). Thus, in the face of such difficulties, many disadvantaged youths focus on the here-and-now because tomorrow is not guaranteed and their long-term prospects for achieving conventional success goals are, to put it plainly, not good, and they know it (e.g., Anderson 1999). As Baumeister et al. (1994) noted, a necessary trigger for self-control is having long-term goals and a commitment to those goals (Kotabe & Hofmann 2015). Without a strong goal commitment, the motivation for self-control is lacking. If one has a foregone conclusion that they will not succeed in reaching long-term goals whether by lack of skill or unfair allocations of rewards, it can be non-adaptive to forgo immediate gratification (Burt et al. 2012; Ellis et al. 2012).

But, fostering unrealistic success goals is not the answer. As discussed above, low self-control tends to beget lower self-control in part through the unsuccessful goal striving that it tends to foster (via negative teacher evaluations, individual frustrations), whereas those students who have higher self-control tend to receive the more proximal reinforcements that sustain long-term goal pursuit (Duckworth et al. 2019). Having strongly held goals and self-efficacy and perceived control over the ability to reach one’s goals is the sine qua non of self-control (Shapiro 1996; Piaget 1962). Deprivation without purpose is both irrational and non-adaptive. Thus, for many, the problem may not be one of lack of control capacity, but lack of hope and control motivation.

But, of course, having realistic goals is not enough. One must be able to keep them in mind and prioritize them against competing impulses; this requires practice, working memory, effort, and time and safety (e.g., Duckwork et al. 2019; Mann et al. 2013). Deliberation and PFC processing is a luxury reserved for those people who are not cognitively overloaded with survival efforts, under threat, emotional duress, among others. In concert with efforts to foster hope and security, ongoing efforts to inculcate and strengthen volitional control skills to help people successfully inhibit impulses in the service of long-term goals along with efforts to foster proactive strategies that avoid temptations in the first place, should be beneficial. If we can help people develop a future to work toward and beneficial habits that facilitate goal-striving, criminal temptations, and thus crimes, may be prevented.

CONCLUSION

In their stimulating tome, G&H (1990) are bold, dismissive, and challenging, characteristics which no doubt contributed to the considerable scholarly attention the theory has commanded. SCT changed the course of criminological research, directing attention to the relevance of internal controls in crime causation, emphasizing the important role of caregivers in influencing levels of self-control, while highlighting the fact that some people suffer the numerous negative consequences of short-sightedness throughout the life-course. However, SCT’s popularity has also facilitated the accumulation of negative evidence for the theory. The very features that most distinguish SCT from other theories of crime and from other models of self-control - its emphasis on parenting being the exclusive cause of individual differences in criminality qua self-control, a critical period of self-control development in childhood, equal high motivation to the immediate benefits of crime, variable recognition of long-term consequences including the recognition that crime invariably decreases subjective utility (provides more pain than pleasure) - are not empirically supported. Thus, in my view, a level-headed assessment based on our current model of science advancement through theory falsification, suggests the need to move beyond SCT.21 No doubt, SCT contains several valuable insights, and tests of the theory have significantly advanced criminological knowledge; yet, the theoretical assumptions are untenable, and the propositions oversimplify and distort a more complicated reality. This combined with the fact that SCT advances a unique conception of self-control, which impairs the integration of several decades of knowledge advances from alternative models, makes the need to move beyond SCT more urgent. In fact, I argue that based on our current stock of knowledge, failing to move beyond SCT would be anchoring, if not regressive, for criminology.

Although it may be tempting to stick with the SCT model that has dominated research, this temptation should be resisted. Moving beyond SCT not only promotes conceptual clarity and integration of empirical findings but also directs attention to crime-relevant individual and situational factors beyond “consideration of consequences”. Underexamined issues from differential motivation to situational variability and domain-specificity cannot be explored within the theoretical framework of SCT, and research advances suggest that these differences exist. Researching these variable processes should put us in a better position to understand how and when self-control failures lead to crime - and how we can best intervene to prevent crime and promote well-being.

Acknowledgments:

Preparation of this review was supported by a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (1K01HD093825-01). I am grateful to editors for inviting me to write this piece and for their suggestions. This article benefited greatly from the multiple readings and counsel of Kara Hannula; my special thanks to her.

Footnotes

1Scholars have argued, for example, that low self-control is one of the “best,” “strongest,” and “most reliable” predictors of criminal and delinquent behaviors (e.g., Pratt & Cullen 2000; Antonaccio & Tittle 2008); “its relationship to delinquent involvement is a ‘fact’ for which extant theories must take account” (Unnever et al. 2003: 483), and “future research omitting self-control from its empirical analysis risks being misspecified” (Pratt & Cullen 2000: 952).

2Hirschi and Gottfredson (1994: 4) raise this question explicitly: “The mystery is, rather, how some people can ignore or misapprehend the automatic consequences of their behavior, both positive and negative, and thus continue to act as though these consequences did not exist”.

3These characteristics include: a “tendency to respond to tangible stimuli in the immediate environment” (impulsivity) and have a “concrete ‘here and now’ orientation” (present orientation); to prefer “easy or simple gratification of desires” and “lack diligence, tenacity, or persistence in a course of action (preference for simple tasks and lack of persistence); to be “adventuresome, active, and physical”; to act “indifferent or insensitive to the suffering and needs of others” (insensitive, self-centered); and to have “minimal tolerance for frustration and little ability to respond to conflict through verbal rather than physical means.” (pp.89–90).

4Indeed, arguing against measures based on the “elements” section, Hirschi (2004) acknowledged that they had erred in this discussion: “we can now see the errors introduced by our excursion into psychology and by the measures of self-control stemming from it” (p.542).

5At least one assessment of this contention suggests that the effects of the Grasmick measure of self-control on crime is not mediated by consideration of delayed sanctions (Tittle & Botchkovar 2005; see also Nagin & Paternoster 1993; Piquero & Tibbetts 1996.) A few other studies which purport to test this key mechanism are limited in that SCT does not argue that people with low self-control cannot consider delayed consequences in cold situations or when asked to do so in response to hypothetical vignettes, but rather that they do not so in the moment when making choices (Piquero & Bouffard 2007).

6Thick description of the lives and focal concerns of youths in dangerous, disadvantaged neighborhoods underscores the reality that for some the choice to carry a weapon or to respond to a challenge with physical force is made against the backdrop of a temporally extended view recognizing the long-term costs of loss of respect from backing down from an aggressive provocation or insult (e.g., Anderson 1990; 1999).

7This is not to suggest that understanding general tendencies is not important or justified. It is. Rather it is to suggest that it appears true, interesting, and potentially important to understand domain- or situation-specific self-control processes and breakdowns given their relevance to criminal behavior. After all, we have legal categories of crime with mitigated culpability due to departure from normal reasoning processes (e.g., voluntary manslaughter).

8In contrast, Clore et al. (1994; also Leith & Baumeister 1996) found that low-arousal forms of emotional duress produce more extensive processing of information, which also impaired self-control in some cases by promoting rumination rather than effective action (see also Agnew 2006).

9Identified by Professor Aikens (credited in Block 1995) as an impediment to scientific progress, the so-called jingle problem refers to the use of the same conceptual label (self-control) to describe different processes. The jangle problem denotes the use of different conceptual labels (self-control, delay discounting, self-regulation) in different models or disciplines to describe the same concept (Kelly 1927). The consequence of the jingle-jangle problem is both the muddying of empirical findings and research inefficiency due to the operation of research on the same topic in parallel, impairing communication and the integration of state-of-the-science knowledge across disciplines into a coherent model.

10Higher order goals are invariably associated with an endorsed end state that motives instrumental action (Moskowitz & Grant 2009). Higher order goals, such as to be a good citizen and honest businessperson, can motivate action that either purely inhibits (e.g., “do not claim fake deductions”) or motivates different action (“do report all of your income to the IRS”; Kotabe & Hoffmann 2015).

11This model pitting immediate gratification against larger-later rewards also applies to punishment (smaller sooner versus larger later), and the response to negatively valanced impulses signals (unease, danger) that have to be overcome for the greater good, such a such as the urge to jump out of the dentist chair or to not get on hot, cramped airplane to get to conference (see Baumeister & Vohs 2004; Hofmann et al. 2009; Trope & Fishbach 2000).

12The concept of self-control as a capacity - or relatively situation-independent stable trait - is not particularly meaningful when broken down into the various components and recognizing situational variability and context-dependence. Much of the debate around the construct of self-control is rooted in the problem of eliding the distinction between behavior, which is shaped by numerous contextual contingencies, and stable(ish) within-the person individual-difference traits.

13G&H (1990) do make a brief note of the fact that their concept is not exactly self-control, remarking that they considered other labels such as conscience before settling on self-control. Even so, the term self-control was widely used outside of criminology prior to the advent of SCT, and numerous scholarly works on self-control processes, including Mischel and colleagues’ celebrated marshmallow studies, preexisted SCT (e.g., Anslie 1975; Ainslie & Hernnstein 1981; Carver & Scheier 1981; Mazur & Logue 1978; Rachlin 1974). Surprisingly, none of these salient works were referenced in G&H’s (1990) tome. Thus, from the start, SCT existed largely in isolation from the broader literature on self-control. It is past time to rectify this situation.

14Ainslie (2001) refers to this as choice instability. Donald’s choice—as a self-control failure—is characterized by choice instability, in that he made a different decision (not to steal) prospectively, before actually being in the store and experiencing the in-the-moment spike in desire.

15By “better characterized,” I mean more usefully, with the aim of facilitating conceptual coherence and consistency.

16I should perhaps note that G&H’s self-control incorporates two distinct cognitive processes: “deliberation” (i.e., time taken to consider consequences) and cognitive extension into the future (or transcendence, defined as “defined as the capacity to perceive the immediate stimulus environment in relation to long-range or abstract concerns” (Baumeister 1995: 122). Given that self-control situations are defined by the clash of situational impulses for immediate gratification against long-term goals, this distinction in self-control conflicts can be ignored.

17What Tittle et al. 2004 called “self-control desire” is tantamount to control motivation. I adopt the term control motivation given the use of desire in this literature as an impulsigenic factor that “directs a person toward immediate reward related stimuli” (Kotabe & Hofmann 2015: 619).

18Executive functions include working memory, task switching, response inhibition, which are supported by prefrontal regions and collectively facilitate control by allowing individuals to keep abstract higher-order goals as active mental representations, suppress undesired thoughts, and focus attention (Diamond 2013; Duckworth & Steinberg 2015).

19This distinction between cognitive, motivational, and affective dimensions is somewhat artificial, as motivations and emotions are, of course, cognitive, and affect shapes motivation, and vice versa. However, it is, in my view, useful to demarcate different facets of this intrapsychic process.

20Specifically, fMRI results showed that, relative to adults, adolescents had significantly greater activation of ventrial striatum and orbitofrontal cortex as they made decisions about risk, but only when their friends were watching them (Chein et al. 2011).

21Reconciling the continued predominance of SCT in light of contradictory evidence alongside G&H’s clear, uncompromising theory is difficult. I hope this fascinating (to me) topic is addressed by sociologists or historians of science.

References

  • Agnew R 2006. Pressured Into Crime: An Overview of General Strain Theory. Los Angeles: Roxbury Press [Google Scholar]
  • Agnew R, Scheuerman H, Grosholz J, Isom D, Watson L, Thaxton S. 2011. Does victimization reduce self-control? A longitudinal analysis. J. Crim. Justice 39:169–74 [Google Scholar]
  • Akers RL. 1973. Deviant Behavior: A Social Learning Approach. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Pub Co. [Google Scholar]
  • Ainslie G 2001. Breakdown of the Will. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  • Ainslie GW. 1975. Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol. Bull 82:463–96 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ainslie GW, Hernnstein RJ. 1981. Preference reversal and delayed reinforcement. Anim. Learn. Behav 9:476–82 [Google Scholar]
  • Akers RL. 1991. Self-control as a general theory of crime. J. Quant. Criminol 7:201–11 [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson E 1990. Streetwise: Race, Class, and Change in an Urban Community. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson E 1999. Code of the Street: Decency, Violence, and the Moral Life of the Inner City. New York: WW Norton [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson SL. 2003. Trajectories of brain development: point of vulnerability or window of opportunity? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 27: 3–18 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Antonaccio O, Tittle CR. 2008. Morality, self-control, and crime. Criminology 46(2):479–510 [Google Scholar]
  • Arneklev BJ, Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ. 1993. Low self-control and imprudent behavior. J. Quant. Criminol 9:225–47 [Google Scholar]
  • Bandura A 1977. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review 842, 191–215 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bandura A 1996. Failures in self-regulation: energy depletion or selective disengagement? Psychol. Inq 7:20–24 [Google Scholar]
  • Bandura A, Mischel W. 1965. Modifications of self-imposed delay of reward through exposure to live and symbolic models. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 2(5):698–705 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Barlow HD. 1991. Explaining crime and analogous acts, or the unrestrained will grab at pleasure whenever they can. J. Crim. Law Criminol 82:229–42 [Google Scholar]
  • Baumeister RF. 1995. Transcendence, guilt, and self-control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18(1): 122–23. [Google Scholar]
  • Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. 1998. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 74:1252–65 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF, Tice DM. 1994. Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic [Google Scholar]
  • Baumeister RF, Tice DM, Vohs KD. 2018. The strength model of self-regulation: conclusions from the second decade of willpower research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 13(2):141–45 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM. 2007. The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci 16:351–55 [Google Scholar]
  • Baumrind D 1966. Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Dev. 37(4):887–907 [Google Scholar]
  • Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M. 2007. Self-control as an executive function: reformulating Gottfredson and Hirschi’s parental socialization thesis. Crim. Justice Behav 34(10):1345–61 [Google Scholar]
  • Bell CC, McBride DF. 2010. Affect regulation and prevention of risky behaviors. JAMA 304(5):565–66 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Benson ML, Moore E. 1992. Are white-collar and common offenders the same? An empirical and theoretical critique of a recently proposed general theory of crime. J. Res. Crime Delinq 29(3):251–72 [Google Scholar]
  • Blakemore S-J, Mills KL. 2014. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annu. Rev. Psychol 65:187–207 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev 108:624–52 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Brezina T, Tekin E, Topalli V. 2009. Might not be a tomorrow: a multimethods approach to anticipated early death and youth crime. Criminology 47(4):1091–129 [Google Scholar]
  • Brody GH, McBride Murry V, McNair L, Chen YF, Gibbons FX, et al. 2005. Linking changes in parenting to parent-child relationship quality and youth self-control: the Strong African American Families Program. J. Res. Adolesc 15:47–69 [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH. 2012. Gottfredson and Hirschi’s conceptualization of self-control: A critique of measures and proposals for change. Unpublished manuscript.
  • Burt CH. 2015. Self-control and crime: a sociological perspective. In The Nurture Versus Biosocial Debate in Criminology, ed. Beaver K, Barnes JC, Boutwell B, pp. 143–71. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Lei MK, Simons RL. 2017. Racial discrimination, racial socialization, and crime over time: A social schematic theory model. Criminology 55(4):938–79 [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Simons RL. 2013. Self-control, thrill seeking, and crime: motivation matters. Crim. Justice Behav 40:1326–48 [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Simons RL. 2014. Pulling back the curtain on heritability studies: biosocial criminology in the postgenomic era. Criminology 52(2):223–62 [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Simons RL. 2015. Interpersonal racial discrimination, ethnic-racial socialization, and offending: Risk and resilience among African American females. Justice Q, 32(3): 532–570. [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Simons RL, Gibbons FX, 2012. Racial discrimination, ethnic-racial socialization, and crime: A micro-sociological model of risk and resilience. Am. Sociol. Rev 77(4):648–77. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Simons RL, Simons LG. 2006. A longitudinal test of the effects of parenting and the stability of self-control: negative evidence for the general theory of crime. Criminology 44(2):353–96 [Google Scholar]
  • Burt CH, Sweeten G, Simons RS. 2014. Self-control through emerging adulthood: instability, multidimensionality, and criminological significance. Criminology 52:45–87 [Google Scholar]
  • Carver CS, Johnson SL, Joormann J. 2009. Two-model models of self-regulation as a tool for conceptualizing effects of the serotonin system in normal behavior and diverse disorders. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci 18:195–99 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carver CS, Scheier MF. 1981. Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control Theory Approach to Human Behavior. Berlin: Springer-Verlag [Google Scholar]
  • Casey BJ. 2015. Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol 66:295–319 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Casey BJ, Getz S, Galvan A. 2008. The adolescent brain. Dev. Rev 28(1):62–77 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Casey BJ, Jones RM, & Somerville LH. 2011. Braking and accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence 21(1):21–33 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Chambers RA, Taylor FR, Potenza MN. 2003. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am. J. Psychiatry 160:1041–52. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Chein J, Albert D, O’Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. 2011. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Dev. Sci 14(2):F1–10 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Chen LH, Baker SP, Braver ER, Li G. 2000. Carrying passengers as a risk factor for crashes fatal to 16- and 17-year-old drivers. J. Am. Med. Assoc 283(12):1578–82 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Chisholm JS. 1999. Death, Hope, and Sex: Steps to an Evolutionary Ecology of Mind and Morality. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  • Clore GL, Schwarz N, Conway M. 1994. Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In Handbook of Social Cognition, Vol. 1. Basic Processes, ed. Wyer R, Srull T, pp. 323–417. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 2nd ed. [Google Scholar]
  • Cohn EG, Farrington DP. 1999. Changes in the most-cited scholars in twenty criminology and criminal justice journals between 1990 and 1995. J. Crim. Justice 27(4):345–59 [Google Scholar]
  • Cross CP, Copping LT, Campbell A. 2011. Sex differences in impulsivity: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull 137(1):97–130 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dahl RE. 2004. Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Ann. NY Acad. Sci 1021:1–21 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dahl RE. 2004. Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Ann. NY Acad. Sci 1021:1–22 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • DeLisi M 2015. Low self-control is a brain-based disorder. In The Nurture Versus Biosocial Debate in Criminology: On the Origins of Criminal Behavior and Criminality, ed. Beaver KM, Barnes JC, Boutwell BB, pp. 172–84. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
  • DeYoung CG. 2013. The neuromodulator of exploration: a unifying theory of the role of dopamine in personality. Front. Hum. Neurosci 7:762. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Diamond A 2013. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol 64:135–68 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dreyfuss MD, Caudle K, Drysdale AT, Johnston NE, Cohen AO, et al. 2014. Teens impulsively react rather than retreat from threat. Dev. Neurosci 36:220–27 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Duckworth AL, Taxer JL, Eskreis-Winkler L, Galla BM, Gross JJ. 2019. Self-control and academic achievement. Annu. Rev. Psychol 70:373–99 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Duckworth AL, Steinberg L. 2015. Unpacking self-control. Child Dev. Perspect 9:32–37 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ellis BJ, Del Guidice M, Dishion TJ, Figueredo AJ, Gray P, et al. 2012. The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: implications for science, policy, and practice. Dev. Psychol 48:598–623 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ernst M, Moolchan ET, Robinson ML. 2001. Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 40(6):630–41 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. 2006. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol. Med 36(3):299–312 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Evans TD, Cullen FT, Burton VS, Dunaway RG, Benson ML. 1997. The social consequences of self-control: testing the general theory of crime. Criminology 35:475–504 [Google Scholar]
  • Fagan J, Freeman RB. 1999. Crime and work. Crime Justice Rev. Res 25:225–29 [Google Scholar]
  • Felson RB, Osgood DW. 2008. Violent crime. See Goode 2008, pp. 160–72 [Google Scholar]
  • Forbes EE, Brown SM, Kimak M, Ferrell RE, Manuck SB, Hariri AR. 2009. Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity. Mol. Psychiatry 14:60–70 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fishbach A, Ferguson MJ. 2007. The goal construct in social psychology. In Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, Vol. 2, ed. Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET, pp. 490–515. New York: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
  • Fishbach A, Friedman RS, Kruglanski AW. 2003. Leading us not unto temptation: momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 84:296–309 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fishbach A, Shah JY. 2006. Self control in action: implicit dispositions toward goals and away fro temptations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 90:820–32 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Forbes EE, Dahl RE. 2010. Pubertal development and behavior: hormonal activation of social and motivational tendencies. Brain Cogn. 72:66–72 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fujita K 2011. On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev 15:352–66 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Freud S 1949. New Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis. London: Hogarth [Google Scholar]
  • Galla BM, Duckworth AL. 2015. More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 109:508–25 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, et al. 2006. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci 26(25):6885–92 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gardner M, Steinberg L. 2005. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev. Psychol 41(4):625–35 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Giancola PR, Josephs RA, Parrott DJ, Duke AA. 2010. Alcohol myopia revisited: Clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 5:265–78 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Geis G 2000. On the absence of self-control as the basis for a general theory of crime: a critique. Theor. Criminol 4:35–53 [Google Scholar]
  • Giedd JN. 2008. The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. J. Adolesc. Health 42(4):335–43 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gibson CL, Piquero AR, Tibbetts SG. 2000. Assessing the relationship between maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and age at first police contact. Justice Quarterly 17(3): 519–42 [Google Scholar]
  • Goode E 2008. Out of Control: Assessing the General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA: Stanf. Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T. 1990. A General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA: Stanf. Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  • Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ Jr., Arneklev BK. 1993. Testing the core empirical implications of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. J. Res. Crime Delinq 30:5–29 [Google Scholar]
  • Greenberg DF, Tamarelli R, Kelley MS. 2002. The generality of the self-control theory of crime. In Advances in Criminological Theory, Vol. 10: Crime and Social Organization, ed. Waring EJ, Wesburd DL pp.49–94. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publ. [Google Scholar]
  • Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. 2008. Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 63(10):927–34 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hay C 2001. Parenting, self-control, and delinquency: a test of self-control theory. Criminology 39:707–36 [Google Scholar]
  • Hay C, Forrest W. 2006. The development of self-control: examining self-control theory’s stability thesis. Criminology 44:739–74 [Google Scholar]
  • Hay C, Meldrum R. 2016. Self-Control and Crime Over the Life Course. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
  • Hay C, Meldrum R, Forrest W, Ciaravolo E. 2010. Stability and change in risk seeking: investigating the effects of an intervention program. Youth Violence Juvenile Justice 8(2):91–106 [Google Scholar]
  • Heckman JJ. 2008. Schools, skills, and synapses. Economic Inquiry 46(3):289–324 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hirschi T 1969. Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press [Google Scholar]
  • Hirschi T 2004. Self-control and crime. See Vohs & Baumeister, pp. 537–52 [Google Scholar]
  • Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR. 1983. Age and the explanation of crime. Am. J. Sociol 89:552–84 [Google Scholar]
  • Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR. 1994. The generality of deviance. In The Generality of Deviance, ed. Hirschi T, Gottfredson M, pp. 1–22[**AU: Just to be sure, this is an chapter in the book with the same title as the book and written by the editors of the book?**]. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publ. [Google Scholar]
  • Hofmann W, Baumeister RF, Forster G, Vohs KD. 2012. Everyday temptations: an experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 102:1318–35 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hofmann W, Friese M, Strack F. 2009. Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 4:162–76 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hofmann W, Kotabe HP. 2012. A general model of preventive and interventive self-control. Social & Personality Psychology Compass 6:707–22. [Google Scholar]
  • Hofmann W, Van Dillen L. 2012. Desire: the new hot spot in self-control research. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci 21:317–22 [Google Scholar]
  • Hope TL, Grasmick HG, Pointon LJ. 2003. The family in Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: structure, parenting, and self-control. Sociol. Focus 36(4):291–311. [Google Scholar]
  • Imhoff R, Schmidt AF, Gerstenberg F. 2013. Exploring the interplay of trait self-control and ego depletion: empirical evidence for ironic effects. Eur. J. Personal 28(5):413–24 [Google Scholar]
  • Inzlicht M, Legault L. 2014. No pain, no gain: how distress underlies effective self-control (and unites diverse social psychological phenomena). In The Control Within: Motivation and Its Regulation, ed. Forgas J, Harmon-Jones E, pp. 115–32. New York, NY: Psychology Press [Google Scholar]
  • Jackson JJ, Ellis BJ. 2009. Synthesizing life history theory with sexual selection: toward a comprehensive model of alternative reproductive strategies. Behav. Brain Sci 32(1):31–32 [Google Scholar]
  • Jacobs BA, Wright R. 1999. Stick-up, street culture, and offender motivation. Criminology 37(1):149–74 [Google Scholar]
  • Jacobsen T, Huss M, Fendrich M, Kruesi MJP, Ziegenhain U. 1997. Children’s ability to delay gratification: longitudinal relations to mother-child attachment. Dev. Psychol 33:703–10 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • James W 1950. The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1. New York: Dover [Google Scholar]
  • Jang SJ, Smith CA. 1997. A test of reciprocal causal relationships among parental supervision, affective ties, and delinquency. J. Res. Crime Delinq 34:307–36 [Google Scholar]
  • Jones RM, Somerville LH, Li J, Ruberry EJ, Libby V, et al. 2011. Behavioral and neural properties of social reinforcement learning. J. Neurosci 31(37):13039–45 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kahneman D 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar Straus Giroux [Google Scholar]
  • Kelley TL. 1927. Interpretation of Educational Measurements. Chicago: World Book [Google Scholar]
  • Klein HJ, Wesson MJ, Hollenbeck JR, Alge BJ. 1999. Goal commitment and the goal-setting process: conceptual clarification and empirical synthesis. J. Appl. Psychol 84:885–96 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kotabe HP, Hofmann W. 2015. On integrating the components of self-control. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 10:618–38 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kruglanski AW, Shah JY, Fishbach A, Friedman R, Chun WY, Sleeth-Keppler D. 2002. A theory of goal systems. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol 34:331–78 [Google Scholar]
  • Leith KP, Baumeister RF. 1996. Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk-taking, and self-regulation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 71:1250–67 Please provide year [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Levitt SD, Venkatesh SA. 2000. An economic analysis of a drug-selling gang’s finances. Q. J. Econ 115:755–89 [Google Scholar]
  • Lin A, Adolphs R, Rangel A. 2012. Social and monetary reward learning engage overlapping neural substrates. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 7(3):274–81 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Logue AW. 1988. Research on self-control: an integrating framework. Behav. Brain Sci 11:665–709 [Google Scholar]
  • Longshore D, Turner S, Stein JA. 1996. Self-control in a criminal sample: an examination of construct validity. Criminology 34:209–28 [Google Scholar]
  • Lowenstein G 1996. Out of control: visceral influences on behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 65:272–92 [Google Scholar]
  • Lytton H 1990. Child and parent effects in boys’ conduct disorder: a reinterpretation. Dev. Psychol 26:683–97 [Google Scholar]
  • Mann T, De Ridder D, Fujita K. 2013. Self-regulation of health behavior: social psychological approaches to goal setting and goal striving. Health Psychol. 32(5):487–98 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marcus B 2004. Self-control in the general theory of crime: theoretical implications of a measurement problem. Theor. Criminol 8:33–55 [Google Scholar]
  • Mazur JE, Logue AW. 1978. Choice in a “self-control” paradigm: effects of a fading procedure. J. Exp. Anal. Behav 30:11–17 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McCarthy B, Hagan J. 2001. When crime pays: capital, competence and criminal success. Soc. Forces 79:1035–59 [Google Scholar]
  • Meldrum RC. 2008. Beyond parenting: an examination of the etiology of self-control. J. Crim. Justice 36:244–51 [Google Scholar]
  • Meloni M 2014. The social brain meets the reactive genome: neuroscience, epigenetics and the new social biology. Front. Hum. Neurosci 8:309. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Meshi D, Morawetz C, Heekeren HR. 2013. Nucleus accumbens response to gains in reputation for the self relative to gains for others predicts social media use. Front. Hum. Neurosci 29(7):439 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Metcalfe J, Mischel W. 1999. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychol. Rev 106(1):3–19 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Milkman KL, Rogers T, Bazerman MH. 2008. Harnessing our inner angels and demons: What we have learned about want/should conflicts and how that knowledge can help us reduce short-sighted decision making. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3:324–38 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller EK, Cohen JD. 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 24:167–202 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Mischel W, Ayduk O. 2004. Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system. See Vohs & Baumeister 2004, pp. 99–129 [Google Scholar]
  • Mischel W, Baker N. 1975. Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 31(2):254–61 [Google Scholar]
  • Mischel W, Ebbesen EB. 1970. Attention in delay of gratification. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 16(2):329 [Google Scholar]
  • Mischel W, Ebbesen EB, Zeiss AR. 1973. Selective attention to the self: Situational and dispositional determinants. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol, 27:129–142. [Google Scholar]
  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Mendoza-Denton R. 2002. Situation-behavior profiles as a locus of consistency in personality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci 11:50–54 [Google Scholar]
  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML. 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–38 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, et al. 2011. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. PNAS 108:2693–98 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Monahan KC, King KM, Shulman EP, Cauffman E, Chassin L. 2015. The effects of violence exposure on the development of impulse control and future orientation across adolescence and early adulthood: time-specific and generalized effects in a sample of juvenile offenders. Dev. Psychopathol 27:1267–83 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Morizot J, Le Blanc M. 2005. Searching for a developmental typology of personality and its relations to antisocial behavior: a longitudinal study of a representative sample of men. J. Personal 73:139–82 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Morrison FJ, & Grammer JK. 2016. Conceptual clutter and measurement mayhem: Proposals for cross-disciplinary integration in conceptualizing and measuring executive function. In Griffin JA, McCardle P, & Freund LS (Eds.), Executive function in preschool-age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research (pp. 327–348). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  • Moskowitz GB, Grant H. 2009. The Psychology of Goals. New York: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
  • Muller C, Sampson RJ, Winter AS. 2018. Environmental inequality: The social causes and consequences of lead exposure. Annu. Rev. of Sociol 44:263–82. [Google Scholar]
  • Muraven M, Baumeister RF, Tice DM. 1999. Longitudinal improvement of self-regulation through practice: building self-control strength through repeated exercise. J. Soc. Psychol 139:446–57 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Muraven M, Pogarsky G, Shmueli D. 2006. Self-control depletion and the general theory of crime. J. Quant. Criminol 22(3):263–77 [Google Scholar]
  • Muraven M, Tice DM, Baumeister R. 1998. Self-control as a limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 74:774–89 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Na C, Paternoster R. 2012. Can self-control change substantially over time? Rethinking the relationship between self-and social control. Criminology 50(2):427–62 [Google Scholar]
  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R. 1991. On the relationship of past to future participation in delinquency. Criminology 29(2):163–89 [Google Scholar]
  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R. 1993. Enduring individual differences and rational choice theories of crime. Law Soc. Rev 27:467–96 [Google Scholar]
  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R. 1994. Personal capital and social control: the deterrence implications of a theory of individual differences in criminal offending. Criminology 32(4):581–606 [Google Scholar]
  • Nagin DS, Paternoster R. 1994. Population heterogeneity and state dependence: State of the evidence and directions for future research. J of Quant. Criminology 16(2):117–44 [Google Scholar]
  • Nurmi J-I. 1991. How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Dev Rev. 11:1–59 [Google Scholar]
  • Nye FI. 1958. Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior. New York: John Wiley [Google Scholar]
  • Ouellette JA, Wood W. 1998. Habit and intention in everyday life: the multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin 124:54–74. [Google Scholar]
  • Pager D 2003. The mark of a criminal record. Am. J. Sociol 108(5):937–75 [Google Scholar]
  • Paternoster R, Pogarsky G. 2009. Rational choice, agency and thoughtfully reflective decision making: the short and long-term consequences of making good choices. J. Quant. Criminol 25(2):103–27 [Google Scholar]
  • Paus T 2005. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci 9:60–68 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pepper GV, Nettle D. 2017. The behavioral constellation of deprivation: causes and consequences. Behav. Brain Sci 40(e314):1–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Perrone D, Sullivan CJ, Pratt TC, Margaryan S. 2004. Parental efficacy, self-control, and delinquency: a test of a general theory of crime on a nationally representative sample of youth. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol 48(3):298–312 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pfefferbaum B, Wood P. 1994. Self-report study of impulsive and delinquent behavior in college students. J. Adolesc. Health 15:295–302 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Piaget J 1964/1968. Six psychological studies. New York: Random House; (A. Tenzer, trans.; original work published in 1964) [Google Scholar]
  • Pickering AD, Gray JA. 1999. The neuroscience of personality. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 2nd Ed. ed. Pervin LA, John OPx, pp. 279–99. New York: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
  • Piquero AR. 2008. Measuring self-control. See Goode 2008, pp. 26–37
  • Piquero AR, Bouffard JA. 2007. Something old, something new: a preliminary investigation of Hirschi’s redefined self-control. Justice Q. 24(1):1–27 [Google Scholar]
  • Piquero AR, Jennings WG, Farrington DP. 2010. On the malleability of self-control: theoretical and policy implications regarding a general theory of crime. Justice Q. 27(6):803–34 [Google Scholar]
  • Piquero AR, Jennings WG, Farrington DP, Diamond B, Gonzalez JMR. 2016. A meta-analysis update on the effectiveness of early self-control improvement programs to improve self-control and reduce delinquency. J. Exp. Criminol 12(2):249–64 [Google Scholar]
  • Piquero AR, Tibbetts S. 1996. Specifying the direct and indirect effects of low self-control and situational factors in offenders’ decision making: toward a more complete model of rational offending. Justice Q. 13:481–510 [Google Scholar]
  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT. 2000. The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Criminology 38:932–64 [Google Scholar]
  • Pratt TC, Turanovic JJ, Fox KA, Wright KA. 2013. Self-control and victimization: a meta-analysis. Criminology 52:87–116 [Google Scholar]
  • Pratt TC, Turner MG, Piquero AR. 2004. Parental socialization and community context: a longitudinal analysis of the structural sources of low self-control. J. Res. Crime Delinq 41:219–43 [Google Scholar]
  • Pridemore WA. 2004. Weekend effects on binge drinking and homicide: the social connection between alcohol and violence in Russia. Addiction 99:1034–41 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Powers WT. 1973. Behavior: The Control of Perception. Chicago: Aldine [Google Scholar]
  • Quinn PD, Stappenbeck CA, Fromme K. 2011. Collegiate heavy drinking prospectively predicts change in sensation seeking and impulsivity. J. Abnorm. Psychol 120(3):543–56 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rachlin H 1974. Self-control. Behaviorism 2:94–107 [Google Scholar]
  • Rachlin H 2000. The Science of Self-Control. Cambridge, MA: Harv. Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  • Rademacher L, Krach S, Kohls G, Irmak A, Grunder G, Spreckelmeyer KN. 2010. Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. NeuroImage 49(4):3276–85 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rawn CD, Vohs KD. 2011. People use self-control to risk personal harm: An intra-interpersonal dilemma. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev 15(3):267–89 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ray JV, Jones S, Loughran TA, Jennings WG. 2013. Testing the stability of self-control: identifying unique developmental patterns and associated risk factors. Crim. Justice Behav 40(6):588–607 [Google Scholar]
  • Reiss AJ. 1951. Delinquency as the failure of personal and social controls. Am. Sociol. Rev 16:196–207 [Google Scholar]
  • Roberts BW, Wood D, Caspi A. 2008. The development of personality traits in adulthood. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, ed. Pervin LA, John OP, pp. 375–98. New York: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
  • Romero EJ, Antonio Gomez-Fraguela M, Luengo A, Sobral J. 2003. The self-control construct in the general theory of crime: an investigation in terms of personality psychology. Psychol. Crime Law 9:61–86 [Google Scholar]
  • Sampson RJ, Laub J. 1993. Crime in the Making: Pathways and Turning Points through Life. Cambridge, MA: Harv. Univ.Press [Google Scholar]
  • Sampson RJ, Laub J. 1997. A life-course theory of cumulative disadvantage and the stability of delinquency. In Developmental Theories of Crime and Delinquency, ed. Thornberry TP, pp. 133–61. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publ. [Google Scholar]
  • Sapolsky RM. 2017. Behave: The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. New York: Penguin [Google Scholar]
  • Scaramella LV, Conger RD, Spoth R, Simons RL. 2002. Evaluation of a social contextual model of delinquency: a cross-study replication. Child Dev. 73:175–95 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schreck CJ, Stewart EA, Fisher BS. 2006. Self-control, victimization, and their influence on risky lifestyles: a longitudinal analysis using panel data. J. Quant. Criminol 22(4):319–40 [Google Scholar]
  • Shapiro DH Jr., Schwartz CE, Astin JA. 1996. Controlling ourselves, controlling our world: psychology’s role in understanding positive and negative consequences of seeking and gaining control. American Psychologist 51(12): 1213–30 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sharkey PT, Tirado-Strayer N, Papachristos AV, Raver CC. 2012. The effect of local violence on children’s attention and impulse control. Am. J. Public Health 102(12):2287–93 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Simpson SS. 2013. White-collar crime: a review of recent developments and promising directions for future research. Annu. Rev. Sociol 39:309–31 [Google Scholar]
  • Simons RL, Burt CH. 2011. Learning to be bad: adverse social conditions, social schemas, and crime. Criminology 49(2):553–98 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Simons RL, Burt CH, Barr AB, Lei MK, Stewart E. 2014. Incorporating routine activities, activity spaces, and situational definitions into the social schematic theory of crime. Criminology 52(4):655–87 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. 2010. A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain and Cognition 72, 124–33 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Steele CM, Josephs RA. 1990. Alcohol myopia: its prized and dangerous effects. Am. Psychol 45(8):921. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Steffensmeier D, Zhong H, Yunmei L. 2017. Age and its relation to crime in Taiwan and the United States: invariant, or does cultural context matter? Criminology 55:377–404 [Google Scholar]
  • Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. 2008. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev. Psychol 44(6):1764–78 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Stewart EA, Simons RL, Conger RD, Scaramella LV. 2002. Beyond the interactional relationship between delinquency and parenting practices: the contribution of legal sanctions. J. Res. Crime Delinq 39(1):36–59 [Google Scholar]
  • Strack F, Deutsch R. 2004. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev 8:220–47 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Stretesky PB, Lynch MJ. 2004. The relationship between lead and crime. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 45(2):214–29 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sutherland EH. 1947. Principles of criminology. Chicago: J.B. Lippincott Co. [Google Scholar]
  • Teasdale B, Silver E. 2009. Neighborhoods and self-control: toward an expanded view of socialization. Soc. Probl 56:205–22 [Google Scholar]
  • Tice DM, Bratslavsky E, Baumeister RF. 2001. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 80:53–67 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Thornberry TP, Lizotte AJ, Krohn MD, Farnworth M, Jang SJ. 1991. Testing interactional theory: an examination of reciprocal causal relationships among family, school, and delinquency. J. Crim. Law Criminol 82:3–35 [Google Scholar]
  • Tittle CR, Botchkovar EV. 2005. Self-control, criminal motivation, and deterrence: an investigation using Russian respondents. Criminology 43:307–54 [Google Scholar]
  • Tittle CR, Ward DA, Grasmick HG. 2003. Self-control and crime/deviance: cognitive vs. behavioral measures. J. Quant. Criminol 19(4):333–65 [Google Scholar]
  • Tittle CR, Ward DA, Grasmick HG. 2004. Capacity for self-control and individual’s interest in exercising self-control. J. Quant. Criminol 20:143–72 [Google Scholar]
  • Toro R, Leonard G, Lerner JV, Lerner RM, Perron M, et al. 2008. Prenatal exposure to maternal cigarette smoking and the adolescent cerebral cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 33(5):1019–27 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Trope Y, Fishbach A. 2000. Counteractive self-control in overcoming temptation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol 79:493–506 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tsukayama E, Duckworth AL, Kim B. 2012. Resisting everything except temptation: evidence and an explanation for domain-specific impulsivity. Eur. J. Personal 26(3):318–34 [Google Scholar]
  • Tsukayama E, Duckworth AL, Kim B. 2013. Domain-specific impulsivity in school age children. Dev. Sci 16:879–93 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Turner MG, Piquero AR, Pratt TC. 2005. The school context as a source of self-control. J. Crim. Justice 33:327–39 [Google Scholar]
  • Unnever JD, Cullen FT, Pratt TC. 2003. Parental management, ADHD, and delinquent involvement: reassessing Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory. Justice Q. 20(3):471–500 [Google Scholar]
  • Ursache A, Blair C, Raver CC. 2012. The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Dev. Perspect 6(2):122–28 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • van Gelder J-L, de Vries RE. 2013. Rational misbehavior? Evaluating an integrated dual-process model of criminal decision making. J Quant. Criminol 30(1): 1–27 [Google Scholar]
  • Vazsonyi AT, Belliston LM. 2007. The family→low self-control→deviance: a cross-cultural and cross-national test of self-control theory. Crim. Justice Behav 34(4):505–30 [Google Scholar]
  • Vazsonyi AT, Huang L. 2010. Where self-control comes from: on the development of self-control and its relationship to deviance over time. Dev. Psychol 46(1):245–57 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vazsonyi AT, Mikuška J, Kelley EL. 2017. It’s time: a meta-analysis on the self-control-deviance link. J. Crim. Justice 48:48–63 [Google Scholar]
  • Vohs KD. 2013. The poor’s poor mental power. Science 341(6149):969–70 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, eds. 2016. Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications. New York: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
  • Vohs KD, Ciarocco NJ. 2004. Interpersonal functioning requires self-regulation. See Vohs & Baumeister, pp. 373–91 [Google Scholar]
  • Western B 2018. Homeward: Life in the Year after Prison. New York: Russell Sage [Google Scholar]
  • Wikström P-OH. 2006. Individuals, settings, and acts of crime: situational mechanisms and the explanation of crime. In The Explanation of Crime: Contexts, Mechanisms, and Development, ed. Wikström P-OH, Sampson RJ, pp. 61–107. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press [Google Scholar]
  • Wikström P-OH, Treiber K. 2007. The role of self-control in crime. Eur. J. Criminol 4:237–64 [Google Scholar]
  • Winfree LT Jr., Taylor TJ, He N, Esbensen FA. 2006. Self-control and variability over time: multivariate results using a 5-year, multisite panel of youths. Crime Delinq. 52(2):253–86 [Google Scholar]
  • Winter AS, Sampson RJ. 2017. From lead exposure in early childhood to adolescent health: a Chicago birth cohort. Am. J. Public Health 107:1496–501 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wright BRE, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. 1999. Low self-control, social bonds, and crime: social causation, social selection, or both? Criminology 37(3):479–514 [Google Scholar]
  • Wright JP, Beaver KM. 2005. Do parents matter in creating self-control in their children? A genetically informed test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s theory of low self-control. Criminology 43:1169–202 [Google Scholar]
  • Yeung N, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. 2004. The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev 111:931–59 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zuckerman M 1994. Behavioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press. [Google Scholar]

What is the low self

The theory of low self-control retains the focus on restraints from engaging in crime but argues that those restraints are primarily internal. People with low self-control, according to this theory, are impulsive and insensitive to others, tend to engage in physical rather than mental activities and to take…

What are the sources of low self

What causes lack of Self-control?.
1) Limited will power. ... .
2) Delaying gratification. ... .
3) Lack of planning. ... .
4) Lack of goals. ... .
5) Decision fatigue. ... .
6) Unsuitable conditions. ... .
7) Lack of discipline..

Which is a characteristic of low self

Given that individuals who are low in self-control are characterized as being, among other things, risk-seeking, impulsive, and angry (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990, p 90), such individuals are likely to place themselves in situations in which the risk of victimization is heightened (Turanovic & Pratt, 2014).

What is the concept of general theory of crime?

The general theory of crime definition explains that self-control predicts hosts among criminals and other people with similar behavior under relatively similar conditions. The general theory of crime reveals that low self-control individuals are highly likely to commit criminal conduct.